News | National
22 Nov 2024 12:24
NZCity News
NZCity CalculatorReturn to NZCity

  • Start Page
  • Personalise
  • Sport
  • Weather
  • Finance
  • Shopping
  • Jobs
  • Horoscopes
  • Lotto Results
  • Photo Gallery
  • Site Gallery
  • TVNow
  • Dating
  • SearchNZ
  • NZSearch
  • Crime.co.nz
  • RugbyLeague
  • Make Home
  • About NZCity
  • Contact NZCity
  • Your Privacy
  • Advertising
  • Login
  • Join for Free

  •   Home > News > National

    Knowing superstitions aren’t real doesn’t stop us behaving superstitiously – why?

    Our new research found behaving superstitiously could benefit us psychologically, even if we know it has no real effect on the outcomes of our actions.

    Stephanie Gomes-Ng, Senior Lecturer of Psychology, Auckland University of Technology, Sarah Cowie, Associate Professor of Psychology, University of Auckland, Waipapa Taumata Rau
    The Conversation


    Former New Zealand prime minister John Key has three white rabbits painted on his helicopter, a nod to his “massively superstitious” habit of repeating “white rabbits” three times at the start of every month.

    Tennis champion Rafael Nadal performs the same sequence of actions (shirt-tug, hair-tuck, face-wipe) before every serve. Taylor Swift paints “13” on her hand for good luck before a show, while Rihanna won’t allow anything yellow in her dressing room.

    Perhaps you, too, are superstitious. Maybe you have a lucky number, avoid black cats, or shudder at the thought of opening an umbrella indoors.

    Even if you don’t consider yourself superstitious, little things like saying “bless you” after a sneeze, knocking on wood or crossing your fingers are all examples of behaviours with superstitious origins.

    We humans are particularly susceptible to superstitions. But why are we so quick to develop superstitious behaviours, and do we really believe they can bring good or bad luck?

    In our new research, we set out to answer this question. We tested whether people could tell the difference between outcomes they caused and outcomes they didn’t cause, and this told us something about the cognitive roots of human superstition.

    Learning about cause and effect

    From as early as four months, infants learn their actions produce outcomes – kicking their legs shakes the crib, shaking a rattle makes an interesting noise, dropping a toy on the floor means mum or dad picks it up.

    Lucky 13: Taylor Swift in concert. Getty Images

    As we grow older, we develop a more sophisticated understanding of cause-and-effect relationships, asking “why?” questions about the world around us.

    This sensitivity to causes and effects sets the stage for important developmental milestones, like imaginative play, planning actions to achieve a goal, predicting others’ intentions, anticipating and regulating emotions, and cooperating with others.

    The ability to learn about relationships between causes and effects is a defining feature of human cognition. But how does this square with our superstitious tendencies?

    When cause and effect is an illusion

    We learn about causes and effects from experience. When our behaviour is followed by an outcome, we learn about the relationship between our action and that outcome. The more often this action-outcome pairing occurs, the stronger the perceived link between them.

    This is why we repeat behaviours that produce rewarding outcomes, and avoid repeating behaviours that produce punishing ones.

    But what happens if an outcome follows our actions by coincidence? If I wear my lucky socks and my favourite sports team wins, this is probably just a coincidence (it’s unlikely my sock-wearing actually caused the win). But if this happens a few times, I may develop a superstition about my lucky socks.

    This suggests superstitious behaviour arises because we aren’t particularly good at discerning when our actions cause an outcome, versus when our actions just coincide with (but do not cause) an outcome. This is a common explanation for superstition – but does it have any weight?

    Testing our ability to detect causality

    We can test what underpins superstitious behaviour by simply asking people “who caused that outcome?”. Getting it right would suggest we can discern action-outcome relationships (and therefore that there must be some other explanation for superstitious behaviour).

    Our research did exactly that. We asked whether people could tell when their actions did or didn’t cause an outcome.

    Cause and effect. Getty Images

    We recruited 371 undergraduate students from a large New Zealand university, who participated in one experimental session for a course credit. Participants played a game where a positive outcome (winning) or a negative outcome (losing) occurred either after their own action (clicking a button), or independently of their action.

    Importantly, participants weren’t given any information beforehand about the type of outcome or whether it would depend on their behaviour. This meant they had to rely on what they actually experienced during the game, and we could test their ability to judge whether they had caused the outcome.

    This also meant participants’ preexisting superstitions and other characteristics (such as age) didn’t affect our results. Their behaviour during the task was representative of human behaviour more generally.

    Participants’ scores indicated they often got it right: in about 80% of trials, they knew when they’d caused the outcome, and when they hadn’t.

    A built-in bias

    The distinction between causing and not causing the outcomes was sometimes very subtle. This made it more difficult for participants to tell what had occurred.

    When they weren’t sure, participants defaulted to saying “I caused it”, even if they actually hadn’t. They were biased to attribute outcomes to their own actions, particularly after winning outcomes.

    This bias may be the key to explaining why we’re superstitious: something I did caused something to happen, even if I can’t be sure what it was. And it suggests knowing superstitions aren’t real may not actually stop us from behaving superstitiously.

    On the surface, this may not make sense – why expend energy doing things we know don’t affect outcomes? But if we look deeper, this bias serves an important purpose, because it helps ensure we don’t miss any potential connections between our actions and their outcomes. In other words, it’s better to be safe than sorry.

    Research shows that engaging in superstitious behaviour can also increase confidence in our abilities to achieve a goal, improve performance in different tasks, and alleviate anxiety by giving us a sense of control.

    The tendency to attribute positive outcomes to our actions (as we found) can boost self-esteem and psychological wellbeing. So, perhaps we’d all benefit by indulging in a little superstitious behaviour. Touch wood.

    The Conversation

    The authors do not work for, consult, own shares in or receive funding from any company or organisation that would benefit from this article, and have disclosed no relevant affiliations beyond their academic appointment.

    This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license.
    © 2024 TheConversation, NZCity

     Other National News
     22 Nov: Dunedin City Council's set to spend almost 350-thousand dollars on its campaign against cuts to the city's new hospital
     22 Nov: Public health surveillance, from social media to sewage, spots disease outbreaks early to stop them fast
     22 Nov: Public transport trials have been cut in several areas in Otago, following NZTA's funding allocation for the region
     22 Nov: Sudan’s national treasures have been stolen – we spoke with the director of museums
     22 Nov: Warriors boss Cameron George claims there was no hesitation taking their Anzac Day NRL game to Christchurch next year
     22 Nov: ICC arrest warrants for Israel’s Netanyahu and Hamas leader doesn’t mean those accused will face trial anytime soon
     22 Nov: A person has been seriously injured after a two-vehicle crash in Auckland's Takaanini
     Top Stories

    RUGBY RUGBY
    Rest during the week - rather than the weekend - for the All Blacks skipper ahead of Sunday's final test of the year against Italy in Turin More...


    BUSINESS BUSINESS
    Dunedin City Council's set to spend almost 350-thousand dollars on its campaign against cuts to the city's new hospital More...



     Today's News

    Entertainment:
    Kendra Wilkinson was "a little bit off mentally" when she was filming her comeback TV show 12:09

    International:
    US to give Kyiv anti-personnel landmines as Russia fires an 'experimental' missile at Ukraine 11:57

    Law and Order:
    Police have already made multiple arrests, 24 hours into the new gang-patch ban 11:57

    Entertainment:
    Liam Payne was remembered as a "son, brother and father" inside his funeral 11:39

    Politics:
    The government has introduced laws for its social media ban. But key details are still missing 11:17

    Entertainment:
    Liam Payne's funeral has sparked a flood of charity donations in his name 11:09

    Rugby:
    Rest during the week - rather than the weekend - for the All Blacks skipper ahead of Sunday's final test of the year against Italy in Turin  11:07

    International:
    International Criminal Court issues arrest warrants for Benjamin Netanyahu, Yoav Gallant and Mohammed Deif 10:57

    Business:
    Dunedin City Council's set to spend almost 350-thousand dollars on its campaign against cuts to the city's new hospital 10:47

    Entertainment:
    Adrian Chiles paid tribute to his friend Liam Payne by wearing a West Bromwich Albion football club jacket to the One Direction star's funeral 10:39


     News Search






    Power Search


    © 2024 New Zealand City Ltd